تعداد نشریات | 161 |
تعداد شمارهها | 6,532 |
تعداد مقالات | 70,502 |
تعداد مشاهده مقاله | 124,117,158 |
تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله | 97,222,283 |
مقایسۀ خودبرتربینی اخلاقی در دو حوزۀ انجام دادن رفتارهای اخلاقی و انجام ندادن رفتارهای غیراخلاقی | ||
فصلنامه پژوهشهای کاربردی روانشناختی | ||
مقاله 11، دوره 8، شماره 1، اردیبهشت 1396، صفحه 157-168 اصل مقاله (470.68 K) | ||
نوع مقاله: مقاله پژوهشی | ||
شناسه دیجیتال (DOI): 10.22059/japr.2017.63461 | ||
نویسنده | ||
ابراهیم احمدی* | ||
استادیار گروه روان شناسی، واحد بویین زهرا، دانشگاه آزاد اسلامی، بویین زهرا، ایران | ||
چکیده | ||
هدف پژوهش حاضر مقایسۀ خودبرتربینی اخلاقی از نظر انجام دادن کارهای خوب با خودبرتربینی اخلاقی از نظر انجام ندادن کارهای بد بود. خودبرتربینی اخلاقی دو شکل دارد: 1- باور به این که «من بیشتر از دیگران کارهای خوب انجام میدهم»،، 2- باور به این که «من کمتر از دیگران کارهای بد انجام میدهم»، و شدت خودبرتربینی اخلاقی در این دو حوزه میتواند متفاوت باشد. طرح پژوهش حاضر آزمایشی و درون-آزمودنی و جامعۀ پژوهش دانشجویان دانشگاه آزاد اسلامی، واحد بویین زهرا در سال تحصیلی 96-95 (پنج هزار نفر) بودند که با روش نمونهگیری تصادفی، 73 نفر از آنها گزینش شدند. ابزار پژوهش یک مقیاس محققساخته بود که در آن از آزمودنیها خواسته میشد که مشخص کنند آنها بیشتر احتمال دارد هفت کار اخلاقی و هفت کار غیراخلاقی را انجام دهند یا دیگران. آزمون t تکنمونهای نشان داد که وقتی آزمودنیها با رفتارهای غیراخلاقی روبرو میشوند، اعلام میکنند که آنها کمتر از دیگران احتمال دارد که آن رفتارها را انجام دهند (خودبرتربین هستند)، اما وقتی با رفتارهای اخلاقی روبرو میشوند اعلام نمیکنند که آنها بیشتر از دیگران احتمال دارد که آن رفتار را انجام دهند (خودبرتربین نیستند). | ||
کلیدواژهها | ||
خودسنجی؛ عزت نفس؛ خودبرتربینی اخلاقی؛ شناخت اجتماعی؛ روان شناسی اخلاق | ||
عنوان مقاله [English] | ||
Comparing ethical self-superiority in the fields of doing ethical behaviors and not doing unethical behaviors | ||
نویسندگان [English] | ||
Ebrahim Ahmadi | ||
Department of Psychology, Buinzahra Branch, Islamic Azad University, Buinzahra, Iran | ||
چکیده [English] | ||
The aim of this study was to compare the ethical self-superiority in terms of doing good deeds and in terms of not doing bad deeds. moral self-superiority has two forms: (1) the belief that "I do good things more than others", and (2) the belief that "I do bad things less than others", and the intensity of moral self-superiority can be different in these two areas. The research design was experimental and within-subjects and the research population were students of Islamic Azad University of Buin Zahra in 2016-17 academic year (N=5000) among which 73 were selected using random sampling. The research tool was a researcher-made scale in which the subjects were asked to determine whether they or others are more likely to do seven moral and immoral behaviors. One-sample t-test showed that when the subjects are confronted with unethical behaviors, they confirm that they are less likely than others to perform the behaviors (self-superiority), but when they encounter ethical behaviors, they do not announce that they are more likely than others to perform the behaviors (do not have self-superiority). | ||
کلیدواژهها [English] | ||
self-evaluation, self-esteem, moral self-superiority, social cognition, moral psychology | ||
مراجع | ||
Bahrami, H., Asadi, M., Shiralipoor, A. (2011). The relationship between identity, self-efficiency, and fundamental needs with general health in school students: A fundamental correlation. Applied psychological research quarterly, 1 (2): 29-44 (in persian). Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323 Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243–267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.243 Bocchiaro, P., Zimbardo, P. G., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2012). To defy or not to defy: An experimental study of the dynamics of disobedience and whistle-blowing. Social Influence, 7, 35–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15534510.2011.648421 Campbell, W. K., & Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration. Review of General Psychology, 3, 23–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.3.1.23 Chambers, J. R., & Windschitl, P. D. (2004). Biases in social comparative judgments: The role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 813–838. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.813 Darley, J. M., & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 377–383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0025589 Dawson, E., Gilovich, T., & Regan, D. T. (2002). Motivated reasoning and performance on the Wason selection task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1379 –1387. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014616702236869 Ditto, P. H., Scepansky, J. A., Munro, G. D., Apanovitch, A. M., & Lockhart, L. K. (1998). Motivated sensitivity to preference-inconsistent information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 53–69. Effron, D. A. (2014). Making mountains of morality from molehills of virtue: Threat causes people to overestimate their moral credentials. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 972–985. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167214533131 Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2000). Feeling “holier than thou”: Are selfserving assessments produced by errors in self- or social prediction? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 861–875. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.86 Flynn, F. J., & Lake, V. K. B. (2008). If you need help, just ask: Underestimating compliance with direct requests for help. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 128–143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.128 Gilbert, D. T., Lieberman, M. D., Morewedge, C. K., & Wilson, T. D. (2004). The peculiar longevity of things not so bad. Psychological Science, 15, 14 –19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501003.x Helzer, E. G., & Dunning, D. (2012). Why and when peer prediction is superior to self-prediction: The weight given to future aspiration versus past achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103,38–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028124 Kawakami, K., Dunn, E., Karmali, F., & Dovidio, J. F. (2009, January 9). Mispredicting affective and behavioral responses to racism. Science, 323, 276–278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1164951 Khodayarifard, M., Ghobaribonab, B., Zamanpoor, E., Derakhshan, M., Akbari, Z., Moosavi, S. (2014). Optimism: Developing an Iranian-Islamic paradigm. Applied psychological research quarterly, 3 (3): 13-23 (in persian). Klein, N., & Epley, N. (2016). Maybe holier, but definitely less evil, than you: Bounded self-righteousness in social judgment. Journal of personality and social psychology, 110(5), 660. Koehler, D. J., & Poon, C. S. K. (2006). Self-predictions overweight strength of current intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 517–524. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.08.003 Kruger, J., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Actions, intentions, and self-assessment: The road to self-enhancement is paved with good intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 328–339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203259932 Messick, D. M., Bloom, S., Boldizar, J. P., & Samuelson, C. D. (1985). Why we are fairer than others. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 480–500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(85)90031-9 Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0040525 Moore, D. A., & Small, D. A. (2007). Error and bias in comparative judgment: On being both better and worse than we think we are. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 972–989. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.972 Myers, D. G. (2010). Social psychology (10th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Pronin, E., & Kugler, M. B. (2007). Valuing thoughts, ignoring behavior: The introspection illusion as a source of the bias blind spot. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 565–578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.011 RahimineZhad, A., Rahimi Jafari, M., Salamati, P. (2016). The relationship between identity styles and cognitive-social-emotional wellbeing: the mediating role of commitment and accepting religious identity. Applied psychological research quarterly, 3 (1): 95-112 (in persian). Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296–320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2 Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 657–661. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2213 Williams, E. F., Gilovich, T., & Dunning, D. (2012). Being all that you can be: The weighting of potential in assessments of self and others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 143–154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211421937 Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2001). From chump to champ: People’s appraisals of their earlier and present selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 572–584. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.572 | ||
آمار تعداد مشاهده مقاله: 600 تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله: 504 |