تعداد نشریات | 161 |
تعداد شمارهها | 6,532 |
تعداد مقالات | 70,500 |
تعداد مشاهده مقاله | 124,085,923 |
تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله | 97,189,485 |
تأثیر سبک پردازش عینی در مقابل سبک پردازش انتزاعی بر ظرفیت حافظة کاری در نشخوار فکری شدید | ||
فصلنامه پژوهشهای کاربردی روانشناختی | ||
مقاله 12، دوره 12، شماره 4، اسفند 1400، صفحه 271-291 اصل مقاله (492.96 K) | ||
نوع مقاله: مقاله پژوهشی | ||
شناسه دیجیتال (DOI): 10.22059/japr.2022.326769.643903 | ||
نویسندگان | ||
بهرام سهیلی فرد1؛ مریم عباسی سورشجانی* 2؛ مسعود غلامعلی لواسانی3 | ||
1دانشجوی کارشناسی ارشد روانشناسی شناختی، دانشکده روانشناسی و علوم تربیتی، دانشگاه تهران، تهران، ایران | ||
2استادیار گروه روانشناسی، دانشکده روانشناسی و علوم تربیتی، دانشگاه تهران، تهران، ایران | ||
3دانشیار گروه روانشناسی تربیتی و مشاوره، دانشکده روانشناسی و علوم تربیتی، دانشگاه تهران، تهران، ایران | ||
چکیده | ||
پژوهش حاضر با هدف بررسی تأثیر سبکهای پردازش عینی و انتزاعی در نشخوار فکری شدید بر ظرفیت حافظة کاری صورت گرفته است. روش پژوهش این مطالعة بنیادی، آزمایشی است و جامعة آماری افراد 50-18 سالة ساکن تهران با نشخوار فکری شدید هستند که به شیوة داوطلبانه در دیماه 1399 در مطالعه شرکت کردند و از میان آنان 30 نفر (21 زن و 9 مرد) با نشخوار فکری شدید براساس پرسشنامههای پاسخ نشخوارگونه (RRS) و افسردگی بک-2 (BDI-II) انتخاب شدند. آزمودنیها با انتساب تصادفی در دو گروه 15 نفری عینی و انتزاعی با تماشای فیلمی غمناک تحت القای خُلق منفی قرار داده شدند. سپس متناسب با گروه خود، دستورالعمل سبک پردازش عینی یا انتزاعی را اجرا کردند. حافظة کاری آزمودنیها پیش از تماشای فیلم و بعد از اجرای دستورالعمل سبک پردازش، بهکمک آزمون فراخنای خواندن (RST) اندازهگیری شد. نتایج تحلیل کوواریانس دادهها با استفاده از نرمافزار SPSS نسخة 26 نشان میدهد، گروه سبک پردازش عینی بهطور معناداری ظرفیت حافظة کاری بیشتری از گروه سبک پردازش انتزاعی داشت؛ بدینمعنا که میزان ظرفیت حافظة کاری تحت تأثیر تغییرات سبک پردازش قرار گرفته است. این تأثیر بر خردهمقیاس پردازش حافظة کاری دیده شد، اما تغییرات میزان ظرفیت خردهمقیاس اندوزش حافظة کاری بین گروهها تفاوت معناداری نداشت. مطابق با یافتههای پیشین، سازگاربودن سبک پردازش عینی در مقایسه با سبک پردازش انتزاعی در این پژوهش نیز دیده شد. نتایج این پژوهش نشان میدهد، سبک پردازش در افراد با نشخوار فکری شدید میتواند پیشبینیکنندة تغییرات میزان ظرفیت حافظة کاری باشد. | ||
کلیدواژهها | ||
حافظة کاری؛ نشخوار فکری؛ سبک پردازش؛ عینی؛ انتزاعی | ||
عنوان مقاله [English] | ||
The Effect of Concrete versus Abstract Processing Mode on Working Memory Capacity in High Rumination | ||
نویسندگان [English] | ||
Bahram Soheilifard1؛ Maryam Abbasi Sooreshjani2؛ Masoud Gholamali Lavasani3 | ||
1MSc. Student, Department of Psychology, psychology and Educational Science Faculty, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran | ||
2Department of Psychology, psychology and Educational Science Faculty, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran | ||
3Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling, psychology and Educational Science Faculty, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran | ||
چکیده [English] | ||
The purpose of this study is to investigate the effects of Concrete Processing Mode (CPM) and Abstract Processing Mode (APM) on working memory capacity during high rumination. The research method of this basic study is experimental. The statistical population consists of individuals aged 18-50 years with high rumination living in Tehran who voluntarily participated in the study in January 2021. Among them, 30 individuals (21 women and 9 men) with high rumination were selected using the Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS) and the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). Participants were randomly divided into two groups of 15 people each, a concrete group and an abstract group, who were put in a negative mood by watching a sad movie. They then performed the instructions CPM or APM, depending on the group. Participants' working memory was measured with the Reading Span Test (RST) before they watched the movie and after they performed the processing mode instruction. The results of the covariance analysis of the data using SPSS version 26 software showed that the CPM group had significantly higher working memory capacity than the APM group. This meant that the capacity of working memory was affected by the changes in processing mode. This effect was evident on the working memory processing subscale, but the changes on the working memory storage subscale did not differ significantly between groups. Consistent with previous findings, CPM was also considered an adaptive mode in contrast to APM in this study. The results showed that processing mode can predict changes in working memory capacity during severe rumination. | ||
کلیدواژهها [English] | ||
Working Memory, Rumination, Processing Mode, Concrete, Abstract | ||
مراجع | ||
اسدزاده، ح. (1388). بررسی رابطه ظرفیت حافظة فعال و عملکرد تحصیلی دانش آموزان سوم راهنمایی شهر تهران. تعلیم و تربیت. 97(25)، 70-53.
فتی، ل.، بیرشک، ب.، عاطفوحید، م. ک.، و استفان دابسون، ک. (1384). ساختارهای معناگذاری/طرحوارهها، حالتهای هیجانی و پردازش شناختی اطلاعات هیجانی: مقایسة دو چارچوب مفهومی. مجلة روانپزشکی و روانشناسی بالینی ایران. 11(3)، 326-312.
فرخی، ح.، سیدزاده، ا.، و مصطفیپور، و. (1396). بررسی ساختار عاملی، اعتبار و پایایی فرم فارسی مقیاس باورهای مثبت و منفی نشخوار فکری (PBRS-NBRS) و مقیاس پاسخ نشخوار فکری (RRS). مجلة پیشرفتهای نوین در علوم رفتاری. 2(14)، 37-21.
Altamirano, L. J., Miyake, A., & Whitmer, A. J. (2010). When mental inflexibility facilitates executive control: Beneficial side effects of ruminative tendencies on goal maintenance. Psychological Science. 21(10), 1377-1382. Antrobus, J. S. (1968). Information theory and stimulus‐independent thought. British Journal of Psychology. 59(4), 423-430. Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 4(11), 417-423. Bassanini, A., Caselli, G., Fiore, F., Ruggiero, G. M., Sassaroli, S., & Watkins, E. R. (2014). Why “why” seems better than “how”. Processes underlining repetitive thinking in an Italian non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences. 64(4), 18-23. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Blagden, J. C., & Craske, M. G. (1996). Effects of active and passive rumination and distraction: A pilot replication with anxious mood. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 10(4), 243-252. Bushman, B. J. (2002). Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame? Catharsis, rumination, distraction, anger, and aggressive responding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 28(6), 724-731. Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., Pedersen, W. C., Vasquez, E. A., & Miller, N. (2005). Chewing on it can chew you up: Effects of rumination on triggered displaced aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 88(6), 969-983. Bushman, B. J., & Gibson, B. (2011). Violent video games cause an increase in aggression long after the game has been turned off. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2(1), 29-32. Carriedo, N., Elosúa, M. R., & García-Madruga, J. A. (2011). Working memory, text comprehension, and propositional reasoning: A new semantic anaphora WM test. The Spanish Journal of Psychology. 14(1), 37-49. Daamen, J., van Vugt, M. K., & Taatgen, N. (2016). Measuring and modeling distraction by self-referential processing in a complex working memory span task. Cognitive Science Society. 31(1), 2147-2152. Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 19(4), 450-466. Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 24(6), 699-711. De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2010). The association between depressive symptoms and executive control impairments in response to emotional and non-emotional information. Cognition and Emotion. 24(2), 264-280. Donaldson, C., Lam, D., & Mathews, A. (2007). Rumination and attention in major depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 45(11), 2664-2678. Ehring, T., Frank, S., & Ehlers, A. (2008). The role of rumination and reduced concreteness in the maintenance of posttraumatic stress disorder and depression following trauma. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 32(4), 488-506. Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion. 7(2), 336-353. Gotlib, I. H., & Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: Current status and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 6(1), 285-312. Hayes M. H. S., & Patterson, D. G. (1921). Experimental development of the graphic rating method. Psychol. Bull. 18(3), 98–99. Hertel, P. T. (1998). Relation between rumination and impaired memory in dysphoric moods. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 107(1), 166-173. Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Gollwitzer, M., Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D. (2005). A revised film set for the induction of basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion. 19(7), 1095-1110. Joormann, J. (2006). Differential effects of rumination and dysphoria on the inhibition of irrelevant emotional material: Evidence from a negative priming task. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 30(2), 149-160. Joormann, J., Dkane, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. Behavior Therapy. 37(3), 269-280. Joormann, J., & Vanderlind, W. M. (2014). Emotion regulation in depression: The role of biased cognition and reduced cognitive control. Clinical Psychological Science. 2(4), 402-421. Klein, K., & Boals, A. (2001). The relationship of life event stress and working memory capacity. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. 15(5), 565-579. Koster, E. H., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review. 31(1), 138-145. Koster, E. H., Hoorelbeke, K., Onraedt, T., Owens, M., & Derakshan, N. (2017). Cognitive control interventions for depression: A systematic review of findings from training studies. Clinical Psychology Review. 53(3), 79-92. Levens, S. M., Muhtadie, L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Rumination and impaired resource allocation in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 118(4), 757-766. Lyubomirsky, S., Kasri, F., & Zehm, K. (2003). Dysphoric rumination impairs concentration on academic tasks. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 27(3), 309-330. Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 69(1), 176-190. Luminet, O. (2004). Measurement of depressive rumination and associated constructs. In C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination (pp. 187-215). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 111(1), 107-123. Marchetti, I., Mor, N., Chiorri, C., & Koster, E. H. (2018). The brief state rumination inventory (BSRI): Validation and psychometric evaluation. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 42(4), 447-460. Meiran, N., Diamond, G. M., Toder, D., & Nemets, B. (2011). Cognitive rigidity in unipolar depression and obsessive compulsive disorder: Examination of task switching, Stroop, working memory updating and post-conflict adaptation. Psychiatry Research. 185(1-2), 149-156. Muris, P., Roelofs, J., Rassin, E., Franken, I., & Mayer, B. (2005). Mediating effects of rumination and worry on the links between neuroticism, anxiety and depression. Personality and Individual Differences. 39(6), 1105-1111. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 100(4), 569-582. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 102(1), 20-28. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L. E., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67(1), 92-104. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 3(5), 400-424. Offredi, A., Caselli, G., Manfredi, C., Ruggiero, G. M., Sassaroli, S., Liuzzo, P., & Rovetto, F. (2016). Effects of anger rumination on different scenarios of anger: An experimental investigation. American Journal of Psychology. 129(4), 381-390. Onraedt, T., & Koster, E. H. (2014). Training working memory to reduce rumination. PloS One. 9(3), https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090632. Owens, M., Koster, E. H., & Derakshan, N. (2012). Impaired filtering of irrelevant information in dysphoria: An ERP study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 7(7), 752-763. Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2001). Positive beliefs about depressive rumination: Development and preliminary validation of a self-report scale. Behavior Therapy. 32(1), 13-26. Pelosi, L., Slade, T., Blumhardt, L. D., & Sharma, V. K. (2000). Working memory dysfunction in major depression: An event-related potential study. Clinical Neurophysiology. 111(9), 1531-1543. Pezzulo, G. (2007). Working memory. Institute of Cognitive Science and Technology-CNR, Roma. Retrieved from http://cogsci.ucd.ie/introtocogsci/docs/ working_memory.pdf Philippot, P., & Brutoux, F. (2008). Induced rumination dampens executive processes in dysphoric young adults. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 39(3), 219-227. Rapee, R. M. (1993). The utilisation of working memory by worry. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 31(6), 617-620. Rimes, K. A., & Watkins, E. (2005). The effects of self-focused rumination on global negative self-judgements in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 43(12), 1673-1681. Rippere, V. (1977). What's the thing to do when you're feeling depressed: A pilot study. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 15(2), 185–191. Rose, E. J., & Ebmeier, K. (2006). Pattern of impaired working memory during major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 90(2-3), 149-161. Rosenbaum, D., Haipt, A., Fuhr, K., Haeussinger, F. B., Metzger, F. G., Nuerk, H. C., …, & Ehlis, A. C. (2017). Aberrant functional connectivity in depression as an index of state and trait rumination. Scientific Reports. 7(1), 1-12. Rusting, C. L., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Regulating responses to anger: Effects of rumination and distraction on angry mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74(3), 790-803. Sari, B. A., Koster, E. H., & Derakshan, N. (2017). The effects of active worrying on working memory capacity. Cognition and Emotion. 31(5), 995-1003. Schaefer, A., Nils, F., Sanchez, X., & Philippot, P. (2010). Assessing the effectiveness of a large database of emotion-eliciting films: A new tool for emotion researchers. Cognition and Emotion. 24(7), 1153-1172. Siegle, G. J., Ghinassi, F., & Thase, M. E. (2007). Neurobehavioral therapies in the 21st century: Summary of an emerging field and an extended example of cognitive control training for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 31(2), 235-262. Siegle, G. J., Price, R. B., Jones, N. P., Ghinassi, F., Painter, T., & Thase, M. E. (2014). You gotta work at it: Pupillary indices of task focus are prognostic for response to a neurocognitive intervention for rumination in depression. Clinical Psychological Science. 2(4), 455-471. Stavropoulos, A., & Berle, D. (2020). The influence of ruminative processing mode on the trajectory of intrusive memories following a negative mood induction. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 68(3), 101528. Stoltzfus, E. R., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1996). Working memory and aging: Current status of the inhibitory view. In J. T. E. Richardson (Ed.), Working memory and human cognition (pp. 66-88). New York: Oxford University Press. Teasdale, J. D., Dritschel, B. H., Taylor, M. J., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Baddeley, A. D. (1995). Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources. Memory & Cognition. 23(5), 551-559. Teasdale, J. D., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and stimulus-independent thought: Effects of memory load and presentation rate. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 5(4), 417-433. Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 27(3), 247-259. Vasquez, E. A., Pedersen, W. C., Bushman, B. J., Kelley, N. J., Demeestere, P., & Miller, N. (2013). Lashing out after stewing over public insults: The effects of public provocation, provocation intensity, and rumination on triggered displaced aggression. Aggressive Behavior. 39(1), 13-29. Wanmaker, S., Geraerts, E., & Franken, I. H. (2015). A working memory training to decrease rumination in depressed and anxious individuals: A double-blind randomized controlled trial. Journal of Affective Disorders. 175(6), 310-319. Watkins, E. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive ruminative self-focus during emotional processing. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 42(9), 1037-1052. Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin. 134(2), 163-206. Watkins, E. R. (2018). Rumination-focused cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression. New York: Guilford Publications. Watkins, E., & Brown, R. G. (2002). Rumination and executive function in depression: An experimental study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 72(3), 400-402. Watkins, E., Moberly, N. J., & Moulds, M. L. (2008). Processing mode causally influences emotional reactivity: distinct effects of abstract versus concrete construal on emotional response. Emotion. 8(3), 364-378. Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: Impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion. 5(3), 319-328. Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. L. (2007). Reduced concreteness of rumination in depression: A pilot study. Personality and Individual Differences. 43(6), 1386-1395. Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: Effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 110(2), 353-357. Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 82(1), 1-8. Whitmer, A. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). An attentional scope model of rumination. Psychological Bulletin. 139(5), 1036-1061. Wisco, B. E., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2008). Ruminative response style. In K. S. Dobson, & D. Dozois (Eds.), Risk factors in depression (pp. 221-236). New York: Elsevier. | ||
آمار تعداد مشاهده مقاله: 659 تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله: 393 |