تعداد نشریات | 161 |
تعداد شمارهها | 6,502 |
تعداد مقالات | 70,280 |
تعداد مشاهده مقاله | 123,510,184 |
تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله | 96,737,734 |
فضای پرت: واکاوی وجوه، تحلیل ساختار فضایی | ||
نشریه هنرهای زیبا: معماری و شهرسازی | ||
مقاله 6، دوره 27، شماره 3، آذر 1401، صفحه 73-86 اصل مقاله (11.34 M) | ||
نوع مقاله: مقاله پژوهشی | ||
شناسه دیجیتال (DOI): 10.22059/jfaup.2022.340950.672755 | ||
نویسنده | ||
صالحه بخارائی* | ||
استادیار گروه معماری، دانشکده معماری و شهرسازی، دانشگاه شهید بهشتی،تهران، ایران | ||
چکیده | ||
فضا محمل ظهور عواطف، شکل گیری تجارب و کنش های انسانی است. ویژگیهای فیزیکی فضا، عامل القاء معانی و ایجاد احساسات در افراد است. فضای رفیع، جادار، دلباز و مطلوب، معانی مثبت فضایی هستند که برآیند توافق جمعی احساسات انسانی است. در نقطه مقابل، فضا بعضا نامطلوب ادراک می شود و به آن صفاتی چون بی روح، ناامن، آشفته، پیچیده و ... اتلاق میگردد. انجام پژوهش به منظور شناخت صفات نامطلوب محیطی به میزان مطالعه در جهت ارتقاء ویژگیهای مثبت فضا اهمیت دارد. در همین راستا، پژوهش حاضر درصدد بررسی یکی از صفات پرکاربرد- والبته نامساعد- فضا است. شناخت وجوه فضای پرت، ضمن وابستگی عمیق به مولفه های اقتصادی، به جهت مغایرت با مبانی حوزه معماری (همنشینی، تناسبات، عملکرد و ...) ضرورت مضاعف می یابد. براین اساس، این مقاله به روش اکتشافی و از طریق واژگان کدگذاری شده مستخرج از مصاحبه با دو گروه جامعه آماری شامل 30 نفر از کاربران پنج آپارتمان مسکونی شهر تهران و 10 نفر از متخصصین و طراحان، به تحلیل مشخصه های "فضای پرت" می پردازد. محصول این تحلیل، تبیین فضای پرت از منظر فضای کالبدی، فضای ادراکی، فضای شناختی و قرارگاه رفتاری و و آگاه نمودن کاربران و عاملان امر طراحی و ساخت فضا نسبت به روش های کنترل این صفت فضایی است. | ||
کلیدواژهها | ||
فضای پرت؛ ادراک؛ پرت آشکار؛ پرت پنهان | ||
عنوان مقاله [English] | ||
Investigating wasted space: an analytic perspective | ||
نویسندگان [English] | ||
Saleheh Bokharaei | ||
Assistant Professor, Department of Architecture, Faculty of Architecture and Urban Planning, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran | ||
چکیده [English] | ||
Studies on environmental psychology include both indoor and outdoor settings. The environment works as an opportunity for emerging human behaviour and emotions. Physical attributes of the environment may affect space meaning and arouse senses. A space is called high, spacious, and preferred because it is attached to such a general rating. On the contrary, one may recall a space as unpreferred, unsafe, complex, and so on. Research has often investigated desirable qualities of environments, but study on disliked environmental attributes is also important, especially when economical components are influential. Since land prices and construction costs are increasing, communities should consider reducing useless spaces. Looking through research, adequate study of “wasted space” was not found. For an exploratory study of “wasted space” and attributes affecting it, 25 apartments were evaluated. These were selected from 55 cases in a high-dense district in Tehran-Iran. Then, a semi structural interview with 30 residents (men, women, parents and children, owners and tenants from 18 to70 years old) was conducted. Each interviewee was asked to define “wasted space,” indicate spaces they saw as “wasted”, explain why they called them as “wasted” and discus their attempts for reusing them. Using the same questionnaire, a group of 10 designers and design professions from Shahid Beheshti University evaluated the same apartments. They also were asked to determine places where they call as wasted space during teaching. Considering different aspects of space (physical, perceived, cognitive, and behavioural) and different features of each, with coded keywords extracted from interviews, research found that: - Both professionals and non-professionals had the same definitions of “wasted space;” - The two main aspects of space for defining “wasted space” were physical and behaviour setting; - For physical space, respondents repeatedly referred to three attributes: geometry, proportion and area;. - For behavioural setting, they only mentioned “furniture;” - Lack of association between the geometry and behavioural settings led to “wasted space”. - Owners and tenants differed in determining “wasted space;” - In some cases, design professionals differed from the others in what they considered wasted space; - Two types of “wasted space” emerged: visible and invisible. Visible referred to physical aspects of space determined almost by both professions and non-professions while invisible type derived from behavioural attributes shown by most of professions. - Factors relating to “perceived space” reduced undesirable effects of “wasted space”. - Affective meaning of space may reduce undesirable effects of wasted space and works as a moderator. - One may wrongly call a space as wasted. A space could formally be perceived as wasted while it may respond to socio-cultural needs. Findings represent a comprehensive structure for wasted space. It can help designers lessen unused space in houses in high-density cities through changing layout, shapes, proportions, juxtapositions and considering behavioural needs. Further study could determine the accuracy and generality of the findings. It could focus on wasted space with other functions such as commercial, educational, and office buildings to know how economical components -which are affective in residential buildings- affect wasted space. | ||
کلیدواژهها [English] | ||
Physical Space, Behavioural Setting, Wasted Space, Environmental Preference | ||
مراجع | ||
آرناسن، یورواردرو، هاروارد،)1374(، تاریخ هنر نوین: نقاشی، پیکرتراشی، معماری، ترجمه محمدتقی فرامرزی، تهران: زرین و نگاه. ( چاپ دوم).
رازجویان، محمود، (1390)، جزوه درسی کلاس نظریه آفرینی دوره دکتری، دانشکده معماری و شهرسازی دانشگاه شهید بهشتی، تهران. هال، ادوارد.تی،(1376)، بعد پنهان، ترجمه منوچهر طبیبیان، تهران: انتشارات دانشگاه تهران.
Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology. Downs, R. M., & Stea, D. (Eds.). (1973). Image and environment: Cognitive mapping and spatial behavior. Transaction Publishers. Evans G. W (1980). Environmental Cognition, Program in social Ecology, University of California, Irvine. Festinger, L. (1962). Cognitive dissonance. Scientific American, 207(4), 93-106. Gibson, J. J. (1976). The ecological approach to visual perception: classic edition. Psychology press. Goodchild, B. (1974). Class differences in environmental perception: an exploratory study. Urban Studies, 11, 157-169. Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation level theory. NY: Harper & Row. Herman, J. F., & Siegel, A. W. (1978). The development of cognitive mapping of the large-scale environment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26(3), 389-406. Kichin M. Robert, (1994). Cognitive maps: What are they and why study them?, Department of Geography, university college of Swansea, Wales SA2 8PP, U.K. Kuipers, B. (1983). The cognitive map: Could it have been any other way?. In Spatial orientation (pp. 345-359). Springer, Boston, MA. Lang, J. (1987). Creating architectural theory. The role of the behavioral sciences in environmental. design. Lang, J. (1988). Symbolic aesthetics in architecture: toward a research agenda. Environmental aesthetics: Theory, research, and applications, 11-26. Lefebvre, H., & Nicholson-Smith, D. (1991). The production of space (Vol. 142). Blackwell: Oxford. Lynch, K. (1964). The image of the city. MIT press. Nasar, J. L. (1994). Urban design aesthetics: The evaluative qualities of building exteriors. Environment and behavior, 26(3), 377-401. Nasar, J. L. (2000). The evaluative image of places. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Neisser, U. (1976).Cognition and reality. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. Newcombe, N. (1982). Development of spatial cognition and cognitive development. In R. Cohen, Ed., Children's Conceptions of Spatial Relationships. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass, pp. 65-82. Rapoport, A., & Hawkes, R. (1970). The perception of urban complexity. Journal of the American Institute of Planners, 36(2), 106-111. Rapoport, A., & Kantor, R. E. (1967). Complexity and ambiguity in environmental design. Journal of the American Institute of Planners, 33(4), 210-221. Rock, I., & Victor, J. (1964). Vision and touch: An experimentally created conflict between the two senses. Science, 143 (Whole No. 3606), 594–596. Sanoff, H. (1971). Behavior settings in residential environments: A research strategy for determining what happens in the designed environment. Journal of Architectural Education, 25(4), 95-97. Schoggen, P. (1989). Behavior settings: A revision and extension of Roger G. Barker's ecological psychology. Stanford University Press. Norberg-Schulz, C. (1971). Existence, space & architecture. New York: Praeger. Thiel, P. (1998). PEOPLE, PATHS, and PURP0SES. Thiel, Philip, Ean Duane Harrison, and Richard S. Alden, (1986).The perception of spatial enclosure as a function of the position of architectural surfaces, Environment and Behavior, vol.18, pp. 227-245. Timmermans, H., & Golledge, R. G. (1990). Applications of behavioural research on spatial problems II: preference and choice. Progress in human geography, 14(3), 311-354. | ||
آمار تعداد مشاهده مقاله: 578 تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله: 751 |